False memories in psychotherapy

1
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
An in Depth Analysis on False Recovered Memories in Psychotherapy
Psychotherapy has a notorious history for false memories. A classic example is the
case of Charlotte Johnson (Johnson v. Rogers Memorial Hospital, 2005). In 1991,
twenty year old Charlotte Johnson accused her father, Charles Johnson, of repeatedly
raping her as a young girl. Later, Charlotte accused her mother, Karen Johnson, of
being complicit to that incest. “The painful memories of the abuse were buried deep in
Charlotte's subconscious and did not surface till she was in adulthood” (Elbow, 2010, p.
1). Charles and Karen Johnson denied all allegations of the abuse and maintained that
mental health treatment providers had implanted false memories of abuse in their
daughter’s mind. Subsequently, in 1996, Charles and Karen sued the hospital where
Charlotte had received her treatment, the Rogers Memorial Hospital in Oconomowoc.
The case went to the Supreme Court. Initially, the Supreme Court ruled that there was
not enough factual information available from which claims of either party (abuse or no
abuse) could be definitively substantiated. In 2005, the Supreme Court reviewed their
decision, ruling that Charles and Karen “could have a limited access to their daughter’s
therapy records to build a factual record” (Elbow, 2010, p. 1). No factual evidence
verifying Charlotte’s claims of abuse have been found.
A similar case was taken to court in 2002, in Monroe, Wisconsin. During
psychotherapy, Marilyn Daly (1992) recovered memories of her father anally raping her
as a three year old. Marilyn further recovered memories of several of her other family
members and neighbors partaking in the sexual abuse. Marilyn claimed that she had
been repeatedly gang raped and had given birth to a premature baby. According to
Marilyn, at the time of her abuse, relatives and neighbors had dressed in long black
2
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
hooded gowns and taken her to a desolate forest. Ritualistic chanting took place around
a fire and her baby was brutally murdered and then fed to animals. Marilyn’s therapists
had insisted that she “recover these memories as a way to relieve her mental distress
(Elbow, 2010, p. 2). The courts (2002) ruled that Marilyn’s therapists had misdiagnosed
her with multiple personality disorder. The courts further ruled that that the therapists at
hand were responsible “for implanting false memories of physical and sexual abuse in
Marilyn, through the use of “hypnosis, guided imagery and age regression” (Robinson,
2002, p. 2).
The above cases of false recovered memories of abuse are not isolated incidents. In
fact, in the 1990s, hundreds of individuals began to come forward with reports that their
therapists had persuaded them into believing that they had been the victims of abuse
(that had actually not taken place) as children. Many of these individuals went on to sue
their therapists for encouraging and fostering false memories. This paper seeks to
reexamine the concept of repressed and recovered memories in light of false memories
in psychotherapy. Whether or not memories can be forgotten and later recovered will be
revisited. The accuracy of such ‘recoveredmemories will further be examined. Finally,
the various causes for the occurrences of false memory in psychotherapy will be
explored.
Sigmund Freud, founder of the psychoanalytical theory of psychotherapy,
maintained that, in an attempt to protect oneself against overwhelming tension and
anxiety, one’s mind automatically suppressed traumatic experiences from awareness, a
concept he termed repression (Freud, 1949). For Freud, the parts of our minds not
consciously aware to us were responsible for most of our neuroses. Freud’s
3
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
psychoanalysis emphasizes on making the unconscious conscious, and Freud believed
that most illnesses of psychological origin could be cured if repressed material was
made conscious. Recovered memory therapy is a branch of psychoanalysis that
focuses specifically on recovering repressed memories. Recovered memory therapy
became a popular trend in psychotherapy during the 1980s and1990s (Brainerd &
Reyna, 2005).
Controversy remains in the psychotherapeutic community over repressed and
recovered memories (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005). Some researchers and mental health
professionals continue to support Freud’s psychoanalytical concept of repression.
These researchers and mental health professionals maintain that the memories of
traumatic events, such as those of sexual abuse, are fundamentally different from
ordinary memories in that they can be encoded in ways that render them inaccessible to
everyday life” (Loftus & Davis, 2006, p. 470). Skeptics of repressed memory, on the
other hand, argue that “traumatic experiences are unlikely to be repressed” and hold
that “if anything, the memories for trauma are stronger than those for ordinary events”
(Loftus & Davis, 2006, p. 471). Skeptics further argue that because the memories of
traumatic events are unlikely to be repressed, the memories of childhood abuse could
not be accurately and reliably recovered in psychotherapy years later.
A study that is often used to support the psychoanalytical notion of repressed
memories is the Williams study of 1994. Williams (1994) conducted a longitudinal study
on 129 women who had documented records of childhood sexual abuse. Abuse had
taken place when the women were between 10 months to 12 years of age. All women’s
reports of the alleged abuse was thoroughly recorded and logged. Participants were
4
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
then followed up and interviewed 17 years later. In interview, researchers asked women
“detailed questions about their abuse histories” (Williams, 1994, p. 1167). Researchers
found that in interview 17 years later, 38% of the women in the study had absolutely no
recollection of any earlier abuse. Interestingly, researchers found that those women
“who were molested by someone they knew were more likely to have no recall of the
abuse” (p. 1167). Researchers concluded that memories of traumatic events can be
forgotten (or repressed) for a period of time, and this does not necessarily mean the
traumatic event did not occur in the first place.
Proponents of repressed memory use the results from the Williams study as
evidence that memories of traumatic events can be forgotten without our awareness.
Skeptics of repressed memory, on the other hand, question the validity of the study.
First, skeptics argue that childhood amnesia would have played a role in several
women’s inability to recall their childhood abuse, not repression (Loftus & Davis, 2006).
Moreover, skeptics argue that interviewers may not have been able to build a good
rapport with all women during the limited interview time, and it is possible that some
women felt uncomfortable or embarrassed in coming forward and disclosing their most
personal memories (Loftus & Davis, 2006). Unfortunately, it is difficult for researchers to
study the repressed memories of real-life traumatic abuse scientifically, as researchers
often have no definitive basis from which the accounts of the sexual abuse can be
factually verified.
The 1997 case study of ‘Jane Doe’ suggests that the memory of traumatic events
can not only be repressed for certain periods of time, but also be later accurately
recovered (Corwin & Olafson 1997). In 1984, Corwin and Olafson videotaped Jane
5
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
while she described detailed accounts of her mother sexual abusing her as a child.
Eleven years later, in 1995, Corwin and Olafson videotaped Jane again and found that
initially Jane had no apparent memory of any childhood sexual abuse. After spending
more time with Jane, researchers found that Jane was able to recall parts of the abuse
she had described to researchers 11 years earlier exactly. Critics have argued that
while the results from Corwin and Olafson’s case study are compelling, the study is
“simply hearsay” and is “bounded by the motivations and interpretations of a storyteller”
(Loftus & Davis, 2006, p. 475).
According to Loftus and Davis (2006), for a study to demonstrate that the
memories of traumatic abuse can indeed be repressed and later accurately recovered
three basic requirements must be met, “(a) that the abuse did take place, (b) that it was
forgotten and inaccessible for some period of time, and (c) that it was later
remembered” (Loftus & Davis, 2006, p. 471). Loftus and Davis argue that studies used
to support the repression and recovery of memories generally do not definitively meet
these criteria.
For some researchers, ‘recovered’ memories of satanic ritualistic abuse provide
the strongest evidence for false memories in psychotherapy (Ofshe & Watters, 1994;
Ross, 1995). The prevalence with which such memories were uncovered during the
1980s and 1990s is outstanding. These researchers maintain that, when evaluated,
such allegations of satanic ritualistic abuse are not only factually incorrect, but are also
often geographically, psychologically, and biologically impossible (Loftus & Davis,
2006).
6
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
Critics have attributed the outburst of false recovered memories in psychotherapy
during the 1980s and 1990s to a number of suggestive procedures that were popular
with therapists at the time. These suggestive procedures include bibliotherapy,
hypnosis, dream interpretation, age regression, guided imagery, and reading old family
photographs. Recent laboratory research suggests that the memories of past events
can be distorted and, in several circumstances, entirely false memories of events, or
memories of events that never took place, can be implanted using these same
suggestive techniques.
To trigger ‘recovered’ memories of childhood sexual abuse, psychotherapists
have often had their clients review old family photos (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005). Lindsay
et al. 2004 conducted a study on undergraduate students in which participants in the
variable condition were administered real old photographs of themselves with
misleading information. Researchers were interested in determining if family photo
albums could be used to cue false childhood memories. Lindsay et al. maintained that
while “old photos might cue long-forgotten memories, when combined with other
suggestive influences they may also contribute to false memories” (p. 149). Participants
in the variable condition of the study were administered their old class grade school
photographs, while participants in the control condition were administered no
photographs. All participants were asked to recall if they had ever ‘put slime ball in their
teacher’s desk’ (the target pseudo-event). Researchers had verified that no participants
had experienced the target pseudo-event before. Researchers found that participants
from the variable group were much more likely to adopt memories of the pseudo-event
than those in the control group. These results suggest that suggestive questioning,
7
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
especially when coupled with factual photographs, can induce false memories of the
past.
Dream interpretation is the psychotherapeutic technique in which therapists
analyze and interpret aspects of their clients’ dreams. Dream interpretation has been
used to recover ‘repressed’ memories of childhood abuse. Researchers have long
criticized psychotherapists for their use of dream interpretation in psychotherapy, and
the validity of dream interpretation remains controversial. In fact, in the False Memory of
Psychotherapy, Brainerd and Reyna (2005) write that “there is no scientific support for
the hypothesis that dreams provide privileged access to weak or forgotten memory” (p.
397). Mazzoni and her colleagues (1999) were interested in determining if dream
interpretations were responsible for eliciting false memories of childhood abuse in
clients during psychotherapy. Researchers created and conducted a study that
examined whether or not dream interpretations could be used to induce false memories
of past events. Mazzoni and her colleagues administered bogus dream interpretations
to a random selection of participants. For example, researchers told participants that the
root cause or the reason for their dreams was that ‘they had been the victims of
harassment in school’ in childhood. Researchers found that “dream subjects were more
likely to increase their belief that they had the critical experience and approximately half
of these subjects also produced concrete memory reports of the event” (p. 125). In other
words, bogus dream interpretations led participants to form false beliefs and memories
of the interpretation researchers had given them. These results suggest that clients are
vulnerable to the dream interpretations their psychotherapists give them, and out of
mere suggestion, interpretations of dreams can cue false memories.
8
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
Guided imagination is another technique psychotherapists have used in
psychotherapy to help clients ‘recover’ memories of childhood sexual abuse. In guided
imagination, the client is asked to mentally think back to a particular time or place. The
therapist guides the client in this process, asking the client specific questions (ex: Is
your mom in the picture? What are you doing? What time of day is it? etc.).
Researchers have criticized the use of guided imagination in therapy, arguing that
asking clients to “actively try to imagine and create images of past events is dangerous”
as “such vivid and elaborate images can later become confused with true memories”
(Loftus & Davis, 2006, p. 484). Libby (2003) was interested in determining if guided
imagination could be used to induce false memories in a group of participants. Using
guided imagination, Libby conducted a study that examined participants’ ‘source
monitoring judgments’ (whether or not participants could distinguish between their
visualizations of imagined experiences and their memories of real life experiences).
Participants were asked to recall past childhood events. Researchers asked participants
‘what they remembered doing’ and ‘what happened to them’ in these recalled
experiences. Researchers then asked participants to visually imagine childhood scenes
with counterfactual information. Participants were specifically asked to “picture the
counterfactual events from the perspective that they had really experienced the events
for themselves” (Libby, 2003, p. 1076). Afterwards, researchers asked participants what
they ‘remembered doing’ and ‘what happened to them’ in their childhood experiences
again, comparing their responses to the initial responses they had given during the
experiment. Researchers found that visualization of counterfactual information did affect
memory recall; specifically, “greater source confusion was experienced when the test
9
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
format encouraged the imagination perspective for representing the real occurrence of
events” (Libby, 2003, p. 1078). These results suggest that when clients are asked to
‘recall’ childhood abuse in psychotherapy using guided imagery, the images the
therapist cues during the process may blur the lines between reality and fantasy
(imagined visualizations).
Like guided imagery, dream interpretation, and old family photographs, hypnosis
was used in psychotherapy during the 1980s and 1990s to help clients ‘recover’
memories of childhood abuse. Using a monotonous tone coupled with relaxation
techniques, therapists helped their clients fall into deep dream-like stances.
Psychotherapists, such as Freud, have long held that hypnosis can act as a portal into
the unconscious mind (Loftus & Davis, 2006). Hypnosis is used as a tool to recover
those memories that have been forgotten and/or that are no longer in conscious
awareness. When used correctly, hypnosis has been known to be an “excellent memory
recovery tool, recovering accurate memories” (Loftus & Davis, 2006, p. 484). However,
hypnosis has also “lead to false memories and confabulation” (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005,
p. 391). Spanos and his colleagues (1999) conducted a study to examine the validity of
‘recovered memories’ under hypnosis. Under hypnosis, participants were induced (e.g.
‘Your mother just brought you back home from the hospital’) to the suggestion ‘of
regression back to the day after their birth (Spanos et al., 1999, p. 201). Researchers
found that 68 of the 78 age-regressed participants ‘recalled’ detailed, impossible, and
false memories of their infancies. Stevenson (1994) conducted a similar study. Led by
suggestion under hypnosis, participants claimed to recall strange memories of their
previous lives (Stevenson, 1994). These results suggest that the memories retrieved
10
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
under hypnosis are not reliable, and should not be used to definitively conclude that
trauma in childhood did or did not occur.
In conclusion, the procedures that were used to ‘recover’ memories of childhood
abuse during the 1980s and 1990s are at best questionable. The above research
demonstrates that the same techniques that were by psychotherapists to ‘recover’
memories of childhood trauma in clients during the 1980s and 1990s can also be used
to cue false memories in participants in controlled laboratory settings. Memories
retrieved by clients during psychotherapy should definitely be viewed with caution.
11
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
References
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2005). False memory in psychotherapy. The science of
false memory (pp. 361-422). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Corwin, D. L., & Olafson, E. (1997). Videotaped discovery of a reportedly unrecallable
memory of child sexual abuse: Comparison with a childhood interview
videotaped 11 years before. Child Maltreatment, 2, 91112.
Elbow, S. (2010, December 21). Memories on trial: Parents say therapists gave
daughter false memories of abuse. Retrieved from
http://host.madison.com/news/local/crime_and_courts/memories-on-trial-parents
say-therapists-gave-daughter-false-memories/article_56549e30-0c89-11e0-a44f
001cc4c03286.html
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psychoanalysis. New York, NY: W.W. Norton &
Company.
Libby, L. K. (2003). Imagery perspective and source monitoring in imagination inflation.
Memory and Cognition, 7, 107281.
Lindsay, D. S., Hagen, L., Read, J. D., Wade, K. A., & Garry, M. (2004). True
photographs and false memories. Psychological Science, 15, 14954.
Loftus, E. F., & Davis, D. (2006). Recovered memories. Annual Review of Clinical
Psychology, 2, 469498.
12
FALSE MEMORIES IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
Mazzoni, G. A., Loftus, E. F., Seitz, A., & Lynn, S. J. (1999). Creating a new childhood:
Changing beliefs and memories through dream interpretation. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 13, 12544.
Ofshe, R., & Watters, E. (1994). Making monsters: False memories, psychotherapy, and
sexual hysteria. New York, NY: Scribner’s.
Spanos, N. P., Burgess, C. A., Burgess, M. F., & Samuels, C. (1999). Creating false
memories of infancy with hypnotic and non-hypnotic procedures. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 13, 20118.
Stevenson, I. (1994). A case of the psychotherapist’s fallacy: Hypnotic regression to
previous lives. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 26, 18893.
Robinson, B. A. (2002, December 20). Satanic ritual abuse. Retrieved from
http://www.religioustolerance.org/sra_rece3.htm
Ross, C. A. (1995). Satanic ritual abuse: Principles of treatment. Toronto, Ontario:
University of Toronto Press.
Williams, L. M. (1994). Recall of childhood trauma: A prospective study of women’s
memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
62, 116776.

Place new order. It's free, fast and safe

-+
550 words

Our customers say

Customer Avatar
Jeff Curtis
USA, Student

"I'm fully satisfied with the essay I've just received. When I read it, I felt like it was exactly what I wanted to say, but couldn’t find the necessary words. Thank you!"

Customer Avatar
Ian McGregor
UK, Student

"I don’t know what I would do without your assistance! With your help, I met my deadline just in time and the work was very professional. I will be back in several days with another assignment!"

Customer Avatar
Shannon Williams
Canada, Student

"It was the perfect experience! I enjoyed working with my writer, he delivered my work on time and followed all the guidelines about the referencing and contents."

  • 5-paragraph Essay
  • Admission Essay
  • Annotated Bibliography
  • Argumentative Essay
  • Article Review
  • Assignment
  • Biography
  • Book/Movie Review
  • Business Plan
  • Case Study
  • Cause and Effect Essay
  • Classification Essay
  • Comparison Essay
  • Coursework
  • Creative Writing
  • Critical Thinking/Review
  • Deductive Essay
  • Definition Essay
  • Essay (Any Type)
  • Exploratory Essay
  • Expository Essay
  • Informal Essay
  • Literature Essay
  • Multiple Choice Question
  • Narrative Essay
  • Personal Essay
  • Persuasive Essay
  • Powerpoint Presentation
  • Reflective Writing
  • Research Essay
  • Response Essay
  • Scholarship Essay
  • Term Paper
We use cookies to provide you with the best possible experience. By using this website you are accepting the use of cookies mentioned in our Privacy Policy.